Skip to main content

Dual task effect on postural control in patients with degenerative cerebellar disorders

Abstract

Background

The cerebellum plays an important role for balance control and the coordination of voluntary movements. Beyond that there is growing evidence that the cerebellum is also involved in cognitive functions. How ataxic motor symptoms are influenced by simultaneous performance of a cognitive task, however, has rarely been assessed and some of the findings are contradictory. We assessed stance in 20 patients with adult onset degenerative almost purely cerebellar disorders and 20 healthy controls during single and dual task conditions (verbal working memory task). To objectively measure postural sway and the impact of somatosensory, visual and vestibular inputs we used static and dynamic posturography with the Sensory Organization Test (SOT).

Results

In both groups, cerebellar patients and controls, dual tasking reduced all sway parameters. Reduction of sway path was higher in cerebellar patients and increased with the difficulty of the postural task. The frequency of falls was higher in the patients group especially during the more challenging conditions and dual task performance in particular increased the risk of falls in cerebellar patients.

Conclusion

Dual task conditions had a larger impact on sway parameters in patients with chronic cerebellar disorders than in healthy controls and lead to an increased risk of falls. As performing two tasks simultaneously is common and therefore important in daily life dual task exercises should be part of physical therapy programs for cerebellar patients.

Background

The cerebellum plays an important role for balance control and the coordination of voluntary movements. Cerebellar degeneration leads to ataxic symptoms, such as ataxia of stance, gait and limbs. Beyond that there is growing evidence that the cerebellum is also involved in cognitive functions [1-3], particularly executive control including working memory and language, and possibly visuospatial function [1]. Cerebellar involvement in cognition is supported by human cerebellar lesion studies and functional brain imaging studies in healthy subjects [4-7].

A deterioration of motor performance especially gait control and a higher risk of falls during simultaneous performance of a cognitive task has been shown for older adults and several neurological diseases, such as Parkinson’s or Alzheimer’s disease [8-13]. There is only little information on the influence of dual task performance on motor execution in cerebellar patients.

An interaction of cognitive and motor functions of the cerebellum is quite conceivable. A recent functional MRI study in healthy subjects showed that during simultaneous performance of cognitive and motor tasks areas in the cerebellar vermis and anterior lobe were additionally activated and had functional connectivity with extensive motor- and cognitive-related regions [14]. So, these regions seem to play an important role to adjust and integrate motor and cognitive networks.

Performing two tasks simultaneously is common in daily life and therefore of high relevance for cerebellar patients. How ataxic motor symptoms are influenced by simultaneous performance of a cognitive task, however, has rarely been assessed, and some of the findings are contradictory. Lang and Bastian [15] examined cerebellar patients and controls in an upright posture while performing a figure-8-movement at the same time as a working memory task. They showed that cognitive and motor performance interferes in patients with cerebellar disorders, and dual tasking worsened motor performance in cerebellar subjects to pre-practice levels. They also provided evidence that the cerebellum plays a role for shifting movement performance from an attentionally demanding to a more automatic state. Ilg and colleagues [16] examined the interaction of working memory and different gait tasks in patients with focal cerebellar lesions. They found decreased performance of working memory and increased tandem gait variability during dual task conditions. By contrast, Salih and colleagues [17] reported an improvement of cerebellar ataxia in a patient with a chronic lesion of the right dentate nucleus when climbing a 10-step staircase and simultaneously performing cognitive tasks (word generation or arithmetic tasks) whereas the same patient needed more time to walk a 10 meter distance when performing a cognitive task. The authors hypothesized that potentially interferences between motor and cognitive cerebello-thalamo-cortical loops can be influenced by specific cognitive tasks and improve ataxic symptoms.

To analyze how cognitive distraction affects stance in patients with chronic degenerative cerebellar disorders, we assessed different stance conditions during single and dual task conditions in cerebellar patients and healthy controls.

Parts of this study have been published as medical doctoral thesis of one of the authors [18].

Results

Sensory Organization Test (SOT)

Typical examples of sway path of a healthy control participant and two cerebellar patients with and without dual tasking during condition 6 are shown in Figure 1. Sway path of the control participant is short with an emphasis on anteroposterior sway during single task and slightly reduced during dual task. Anteroposterior and mediolateral sway path of the cerebellar patient is increased compared to the control during both conditions, single and dual task, and decreases during dual tasking. The last example shows sway path of a cerebellar patient before a fall with increased sway in anteroposterior and mediolateral direction during dual task.

Figure 1
figure 1

Sway path of center of gravity in condition 6 comparing single and dual task conditions for a healthy control and two cerebellar patients; modified according to [18].

Cerebellar patients showed higher sway area (p = 0.002) and anteroposterior sway (p = 0.001) than controls [significant effect for group; 2 Groups (controls vs. cerebellar) x 2 Sets (single vs. dual task) x 6 Sway Conditions (6 SOT conditions) ANOVA] (Figure 2; Table 1). Group differences were close to significance considering sway path (p = 0.057) and mediolateral sway (p = 0.057). In both groups sway area (p < 0.001), sway path (p < 0.001), anteroposterior (p < 0.001) and mediolateral sway (p < 0.001) differed between the six different conditions (effect of condition; Table 1) and tended to show highest values in condition 5 and 6 (Figure 2). For sway area (p = 0.005) but not for the other sway parameters, the difference between the conditions was higher in cerebellar patients than in healthy controls (significant group by condition interaction; Table 1).

Figure 2
figure 2

Sway parameters of patients and controls during single and dual task for all stance conditions (given as mean values with standard deviation); modified according to [18]. Sway parameters of cerebellar patients are marked green (dark green single and light green dual task), of controls grey (dark grey single and light grey dual task).

Table 1 Analysis of variance of sway measures

In both groups, cerebellar patients and controls, dual task significantly reduced sway area (p < 0.001), sway path (p < 0.001), anteroposterior sway (p < 0.001) and mediolateral sway (p = 0.002) compared to the single task (Table 1; Figure 2). Effects of dual task did not differ between the different conditions (dual task by condition interaction) (Table 1, Figure 2).

Overall the reduction did not differ between the cerebellar and the control group (no significant dual task by group interaction). Taking into account the different conditions, we found that cerebellar patients showed significantly less sway path during dual task during the more advanced conditions (p = 0.007; group by dual task by condition interaction), this interaction was not significant for sway area, anteroposterior and mediolateral sway (Table 1).

The number of falls in all trials and conditions were counted per patient. Frequency of falls was different between conditions (p < 0.001; effect of condition; 2 Groups (controls vs. cerebellar) x 2 Sets (single vs. dual task) x 6 Sway Conditions (6 SOT conditions) ANOVA) and significantly higher in the patients’ group (p < 0.001; group effect): While there was only one fall in condition 5 in the control group, falls were frequent in the patient group (43 falls during single task, 50 falls during dual task conditions), mainly during conditions 5 and 6 (86 of 93 falls) (Figure 3). In cerebellar patients dual tasking also increased the risk of falls with increasing difficulty of the conditions (p < 0.001; group by dual task by condition interaction).

Figure 3
figure 3

Number of falls of patients during single and dual task conditions for all stance conditions (given as mean values with standard deviation); modified according to [18]. Number of falls of cerebellar patients is marked green (dark green single and light green dual task).

Errors in the cognitive task were calculated as percentage error of all target letters to be counted. The mean error was numerically higher in the patient group (75 vs. 39 miscounted letters in patients vs. controls), but this difference was not significant (p = 0.105; 2 Groups (controls vs. cerebellar) x 2 Sets (single vs. dual task) x 6 Sway Conditions (6 SOT conditions) ANOVA) (Figure 4). However, because falls of cerebellar patients during condition 5 and 6 were so frequent and patients stopped counting, these trials had to be excluded from this analysis. Hence error ratio in the patient group actually underestimates the effect of the dual task on memory error.

Figure 4
figure 4

Percentage error in the cognitive task during dual task for all stance conditions (given as mean values with standard deviation); modified according to [18]. Percentage error of cerebellar patients is marked green, of controls grey.

Discussion

In this study we assessed stance in patients with chronic cerebellar disorders and healthy controls during single and dual task conditions to see how a simultaneous cognitive task affects balance. In both groups, cerebellar patients and controls, dual tasking reduced all sway parameters. Reduction of sway path was higher in cerebellar patients and increased with the difficulty of the postural task. The frequency of falls was higher in the patients group especially during the more advanced conditions. In cerebellar patients dual task performance increased the risk of falls.

As expected, cerebellar patients showed ataxic stance with higher sway parameters than controls in all observed conditions. Deficits became most apparent in condition 5 and 6, those conditions in which platform tilt and/or vision were sway-referenced. Therefore quality of stance mainly depends on the vestibular or vestibulocerebellar system. This is in line with the results of Gatev and colleagues [19] who assessed 25 patients with cortical cerebellar atrophy (CCA), nine with olivo-ponto-cerebellar atrophy (OPCA) and 10 healthy controls with the Sensory Organization Test. They reported more body sway as well as higher frequency of falls in the patients’ group and in general more sway during condition 5 and 6.

The main aim of our study was to examine how cognitive distraction affected motor performance in patients with chronic cerebellar disorders. In both groups, cerebellar patients and controls, dual task reduced all sway parameters. Taking into account the different conditions, we found that cerebellar patients showed a significant reduction of sway path during dual task in the more advanced conditions. On the other hand dual task increased the risk of falls during the more advanced conditions. Decrease of sway might be due to higher co-contractions of antagonistic muscle groups during postural adjustments to compensate ataxia [20]. Cerebellar patients often show increased muscle stiffness [21]. This is may also reflect a primary cerebellar deficit. Increased co-contraction is supposed to decrease the ability to react to challenging balance leading to an increased risk of falls. In fact, physiotherapy in cerebellar patients aims at decreasing co-contractions and compensatory stiffening of limbs. The efficacy of such physiotherapeutic interventions that train dynamic stability of stance and gait has recently been shown [22,23].

Our observation that dual task worsened motor performance in cerebellar subjects is in line with the results of Lang and Bastian [15] who examined cerebellar patients and controls with an upper limb motor task and Ilg and colleagues [16] who examined the gait of cerebellar patients and controls under dual task conditions. Our results are at variance with findings of Salih et al. [17] who found a partial improvement of ataxic symptoms during the concurrent performance of a cognitive task. One may argue that decreased sway under dual task conditions reflects an improvement of postural parameters in cerebellar patients in the present study. However, as discussed above, this may also be interpreted as a sign of malcompensation leading to the observed increased number of falls.

A deterioration of motor performance during dual task conditions has been shown for older adults and other neurological conditions, such as Parkinson’s or Alzheimer’s disease [10-13]. Furthermore combining physical and cognitive training has been shown to be more beneficial in improving gait stability and preventing falls in older adults [8,9]. Performing a motor and a cognitive task simultaneously potentially diminishes the attention available for stance control. This may partly explain the changes of sway parameters in both, cerebellar patients and healthy controls. Changes, however, were more prominent in the more difficult conditions in cerebellar patients suggesting an additional specific cerebellar effect on motor performance during dual tasking. There is increasing evidence that the cerebellum not only plays an important role in motor control but is also involved in cognitive processes [1,24-27]. In particular, it has been shown that the cerebellum plays a role in verbal working memory [28,29]. More specifically, a recent functional MRT study suggested that the cerebellum is directly involved in dual task performance by adjusting and integrating motor and cognitive networks [14].

Errors in the working memory task were numerically higher in the patients’ group, but this difference was not significant. This does not contradict the literature, because overall, verbal working memory deficits tend to be mild in adult patients with chronic cerebellar disorders [25]. However, error ratio in the patients group might have been misleadingly low, because falls of cerebellar patients were frequent, especially during the more advanced conditions and patients stopped counting the letters, so these trials had to be excluded from the analysis. Likewise, we cannot exclude that errors increased in the more advanced postural conditions. Furthermore, working memory task had not been assessed while sitting or lying, and the effect of unperturbed stance on cognitive performance could not be assessed. A recent dual task study in cerebellar patients reported an influence of motor task complexity on the performance in a working memory task [16].

Conclusion

In summary sway parameters during dual task conditions were changed in patients with chronic cerebellar disorders and lead to an increased risk of falls. As performing two tasks simultaneously is common and therefore important in daily life dual task exercises should be part of physical therapy programs for cerebellar patients.

Methods

Participants

A group of 20 patients with adult onset degenerative almost purely cerebellar disorders [sporadic adult onset ataxia (SAOA): 12; spinocerebellar ataxia type 6 (SCA6): 3; auto-immune cerebellar ataxia with GAD-antibodies: 2; purely cerebellar autosomal dominant ataxia (ADCA-III): 2; cerebellitis: 1] and 20 age-matched healthy controls [58.4 ± 11.64 years vs 58.25 ± 11.66 years (mean age ± SD)] were included in the study. Both groups consisted of 10 female and 10 male persons. Severity of ataxia was minor to moderate [ICARS 26.7 ± 11.2; SARA-score 10.2 ± 4.4 (mean ± SD)].

Participants were consecutively recruited within a period between March and July 2010. Inclusion criteria were progressive, almost purely cerebellar ataxia (patients) and age between 18 and 75 years (both groups). Exclusion criteria were other neurological diseases or symptoms other than cerebellar (patients), neurological or orthopaedic diseases (healthy controls) and the intake of sedatives (excluding low-dose treatment of antidepressants).

Ethics, consent and permissions

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The study has approved by the local ethics committee (09–4170). Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Assessment instruments

The severity of ataxia was assessed with the Scale for the Assessment and Rating of Ataxia (SARA), an 8-item clinical rating scale ranging from 0 (no ataxia) to 40 (most severe ataxia) [30] and the International Cooperative Ataxia Rating Scale (ICARS), a 19-items clinical rating scale ranging from 0 (no ataxia) to 100 (most severe ataxia) [31]. The investigator (DT) was trained in the application of the applied scales.

Static and dynamic posturography was performed with the Sensory Organization Test (SOT) using the EquiTest system (neuroCom, Inc., USA) to objectively measure postural sway and the impact of somatosensory, visual and vestibular inputs under six different visual and support-surface conditions: 1. Eyes open, fixed platform surface and background, 2. Eyes closed, fixed platform surface and background, 3. Eyes open, fixed platform surface and sway-referenced visual background, 4. Eyes open and sway-referenced surface, 5. Eyes closed and sway-referenced surface and 6. Eyes open, sway-referenced surface and visual background. Stance width was fixed in all subjects. Ground reaction forces were recorded for 20 seconds with a sampling rate of 100 Hertz. The SOT consisted of 18 trials in total, 3 trials per postural condition. The postural test system recorded data of the four force transducers and the associated shear forces between the two force plates. With customized software based on MATLAB technical programming language, the raw data were calibrated and subsequently filtered offline using a 4th-order lowpass Butterworth filter with a cut-off frequency of 3 Hz. Based on the filtered force-time data of each 20 s trial, the following postural variables were computed: Sway path length defined as the total length of the path participant’s centre of gravity, sway area as area between the maximum excursions of a participant’s centre of gravity along the mediolateral (ML) and anterioposterior (AP) axes. Furthermore, the range of sway in the AP and ML direction was calculated.

All six stance conditions were tested in a single and dual task paradigm. In the single task treatment, participants performed the standard SOT test, in the dual task paradigm, they performed an additional verbal working memory task. During the verbal memory task participants listened to a 20-letter-sequence at one Hz frequency, that consisted of a random series of four letters (A, G, M, O). Participants were asked to count the number of a target letter in every sequence. The sequence and target letter changed every trial. Percentage error was calculated with the maximum number of letters to be counted defined as 100 percent. Each participant completed two sets of the Sensory Organisation Test. During the first set, the first two trials per condition were performed without cognitive distraction (single task), the last of the three trials per condition under dual task conditions (condition 1: single task, single task, dual task; condition 2: single task, single task, dual task; etc.). During the second set the first trial per condition was performed without cognitive distraction (single task), the second trial under dual task conditions and the third trial again without cognitive distraction (single task) (condition 1: single task, dual task, single task; condition 2: single task, dual task, single task; etc.).

Statistical analysis

For each of the six different conditions the first trial of the first set (single task) and the last trial of the second set (single task) were excluded from the final analysis. Therefore, four trials per condition (two under single and two under dual task conditions) were analysed and arithmetic means were compared. For each sway parameter (sway path, sway area, anteroposterior (AP) and mediolateral (ML) sway) a 2 Groups x 2 Sets x 6 Sway Conditions repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Bonferroni correction was performed. P values < 0.0125 were considered significant (that is 0.05 divided by four, the number of ANOVAs). Falls were excluded from the analysis of sway parameters and analysed separately with an analysis of variance (ANOVA). Analyses were performed with IBM SPSS 18.0 (SPSS Inc., 2009).

References

  1. Timmann D, Daum I. Cerebellar contributions to cognitive functions: a progress report after two decades of research. Cerebellum. 2007;6:159–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Molinari M, Filipini V, Leggio MG. Neuronal plasticity of interrelated cerebellar and cortical networks. Neuroscience. 2002;111(4):863–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Schmahmann JD, Pandya DN. The cerebrocerebellar system. Int Rev Neurobiol. 1997;41:31–60.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. KH E, Chen SH, Ho MH, Desmond JE. A meta-analysis of cerebellar contributions to higher cognition from PET and fMRI studies. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35(2):593–615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Stoodley CJ, Schmahmann JD. Functional topography in the human cerebellum: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuroimage. 2009;44(2):489–501.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Ravizza SM, McCormick CA, Schlerf JE, Justus T, Ivry RB, Fiez JA. Cerebellar damage produces selective deficits in verbal working memory. Brain. 2006;129:306–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Chen SH, Desmond JE. Cerebrocerebellar networks during articulatory rehearsal and verbal working memory tasks. NeuroImage. 2005;24(2):332–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. van Het Reve E, de Bruin ED. Strength-balance supplemented with computerized cognitive training to improve dual task gait and divided attention in older adults: a multicenter randomized-controlled trial. BMC Geriatr. 2014;14(1):134.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Zijlstra A, Ufkes T, Skelton DA, Lundin-Olsson L, Zijlstra W. Do dual tasks have an added value over single tasks for balance assessment in fall prevention programs? A mini-review. Gerontology. 2008;54(1):40–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Yogev G, Giladi N, Peretz C, Springer S, Simon ES, Hausdorff JM. Dual tasking, gait rhytmicity, and Parkinson’s disease: Which aspects of gate are attention demanding? J Neurosci. 2005;22(5):1248–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Marchese R, Bove M, Abbruzzese G. Effect of cognitive and motor tasks on postural stability in Parkinson’s disease: a posturographic study. Mov Disord. 2003;18(6):652–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Sheridan PL, Solomont J, Kowall N, Hausdorff JM. Influence of executive function on locomotor function: divided attention increases gait variability in Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51(11):1633–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Shumway-Cook A, Woollacott M, Kerns KA, Baldwin M. The effects of two types of cognitive task on postural stability in older adults with and without a history of falls. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 1997;52(4):M232–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Wu T, Liu J, Hallett M, Zheng Z, Chan P. Cerebellum and integration of neural networks in dual-task processing. Neuroimage. 2013;15(65):466–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Lang CE, Bastian AJ. Cerebellar damage impairs automaticity of a recently practiced movement. J Neurophysiol. 2002;87(3):1336–47.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Ilg W, Christensen A, Mueller OM, Goericke SL, Giese MA, Timmann D. Effects of cerebellar lesions on working memory interacting with motor tasks of different complexities. J Neurophysiol. 2013;110(10):2337–49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Salih F, Breuer E, Harnack D, Hoffmann K-T, Ploner CJ. A syndrome of the dentate nucleus mimicking psychogenic ataxia. J Neurol Sci. 2010;290(1–2):183–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Alfes, JV. Standsicherheit in einer Dual-Task-Aufgabe bei Patienten mit degenerativen Kleinhirnerkrankungen. 2013; 91 Bl.. Duisburg, Essen, Univ., Diss., 2013. http://d-nb.info/104601255X

  19. Gatev P, Thomas S, Lou JS, Lim M, Hallet M. Effects of diminished and conflicting sensory information on balance in patients with cerebellar deficits. Mov Disord. 1996;11(6):654–64.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Asaka T, Wang Y. Feedforward postural muscle modes and multi-mode coordination in mild cerebellar ataxia. Exp Brain Resp. 2011;210(1):153–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Küng UM, Horlings CG, Honegger F, Kremer HP, Bloem BR, van De Warrenburg BP, et al. Postural instability in cerebellar ataxia: correlations of knee, arm and trunk movements to center of mass velocity. Neuroscience. 2009;159(1):390–404.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Ilg W, Broetz D, Burkard S, Giese MA, Schöls L, Synofzik M. Long-term effects of coordinative training in degenerative cerebellar disease. Mov Disord. 2010;25(13):2239–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Ilg W, Synofzik M, Brötz D, Burkard S, Giese MA, Schöls L. Intensive coordinative training improves motor performance in degenerative cerebellar disease. Neurology. 2009;73(22):1823–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Stoodley CJ, Schmahmann JD. Evidence for topographic organization in the cerebellum of motor control versus cognitive and affective processing. Cortex. 2010;46:831–44.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Timmann D, Daum I. How consistent are cognitive impairments in patients with cerebellar disorders? Behav Neurol. 2010;23:81–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Strick PL, Dum RP, Fiez JA. Cerebellum and nonmotor function. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2009;32:413–34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Ito M. Control of mental activities by internal models in the cerebellum. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(4):304–13.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Durisko C, Fiez JA. Functional activation in the cerebellum during working memory and simple speech tasks. Cortex. 2010;46(7):896–906.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Desmond JE, Fiez JA. Neuroimaging studies of the cerebellum: language, learning and memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 1998;2(9):355–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Schmitz-Hübsch T, Tezenas du Montcel S, Baliko L, Berciano J, Boesch S, Depondt C. Scale for the assessment and rating of ataxia: development of a new clinical scale. Neurology. 2006;66:1717–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Trouillas P, Takayanagi T, Hallett M, Currier RD, Subramony SH, Wessel K, et al. International Cooperative Ataxia Rating Scale for pharmacological assessment of the cerebellar syndrome. The Ataxia Neuropharmacology Committee of the World Federation of Neurology. J Neurol Sci. 1997;145(2):205–11.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgement

We thank all patients for participation in the study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Heike Jacobi.

Additional information

Competing interests

H. Jacobi: None.

J. Alfes: None.

M. Minnerop: None.

J. Konczak: None.

T. Klockgether receives/has received research support from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG), the Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung (BMBF) and the European Union (EU). He serves on the editorial board of The Cerebellum. He received a lecture honorarium from Lundbeck and from Biogen Idec. He receives royalties for book publications from Thieme, Urban & Schwarzenberg, Kohlhammer, Elsevier, Wissenschaftliche Verlagsgesellschaft Stuttgart and M. Dekker.

D. Timmann received research support from the German Research Foundation (DFG TI 239/10-1; 10–2; 14–1), the European Union (Partner in one of the Marie Curie Initial Training Networks), the Bernd Fink Foundation, and the German Heredoataxia Foundation.

Authors’ contributions

HJ, DT and TK conceived of the study, and participated in its design and coordination. JK participated in the design of the study. JA carried out the patients’ assessment. MM participated in the analysis of patient data. JA and DT performed the statistical analysis. HJ drafted the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Rights and permissions

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Jacobi, H., Alfes, J., Minnerop, M. et al. Dual task effect on postural control in patients with degenerative cerebellar disorders. cerebellum ataxias 2, 6 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40673-015-0025-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40673-015-0025-z

Keywords